Слайд 2Outline
Introduction
Visual illusions
Pictorial perception
4. Intelligence across cultures
4.1 General
Intelligence ‘g’
4.2 Indigenous conceptions
5. Cognitive styles:
5.1 Field dependence/independence
5.2 East/ West styles
6. Conclusions
Слайд 3 Readings
Berry, et al (2011). Cross-cultural Psychology. Chapters 9 and 6.
Segall,
M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovits, K. J. (1966). The influence of culture on visual perception. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.
Berry, J.W., and Dasen, P. (1974) (Eds.). Culture and Cognition, London: Methuen found
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently … and why. New York: The Free Press
Слайд 41. Introduction
The field of study called culture and cognition includes a
number of related phenomena: sensation, perception, intelligence and cognitive style.
Sensation and perception were one of the earliest areas of psychology to be examined across cultures.
Слайд 51. Introduction
The 1899 expedition to the Torres Strait Islands by Rivers
examined a number of phenomena, including colour perception and the susceptibility of these peoples to visual illusions.
The belief at that time was that these ‘savages’ would be tricked more easily that would Cambridge undergraduates.
However, the findings were more complicated!
Слайд 82. Visual Illusion Susceptibility
One of the classics of ccp are studies
of susceptibility to visual illusions (Segall, Campbell & Herskovits, 1962, 1966)
They examined 2 theory-derived hypotheses, checked the relevant (external) cultural conditions and analyzed their data
They had data collected by colleagues around the world [in 14 non-western and 3 western contexts], using a well-designed stimulus book.
The two hypotheses were:
carpentered world hypothesis;
foreshortening hypothesis
Both hypotheses are based on the view that we respond to visual illusions on the basis of what we have learned in our visual ecology
Слайд 92. Carpentered World Hypothesis
Susceptibility to angled illusions [such as the Muller-Lyer
arrows, and the Sander parallelogram] is promoted by living in visual environments that are carpentered to produce many right angles.
Perceivers are ‘tricked’ by interpreting these acute and obtuse angles as right angles.
In the ML, they overestimate the length of the line subtended by the outward pointing arrows
In the Sander, they over estimate the length of the line subtended between the obtuse angles
Слайд 10© Cambridge University Press 2011
Mueller-Lyer illusion (carpentered world hypothesis)
Слайд 11© Cambridge University Press 2011
Sander parallelogram (carpentered world hypothesis)
Слайд 122. Foreshortening Hypothesis
Susceptibility to the horizontal-vertical illusion is promoted for people
who experience long distances in a horizontal plane, and having few verticals.
Perceivers are ‘tricked’ into interpreting a vertical stimulus as a long line running away from them in the horizontal plane.
They thus overestimate the length of the vertical line in relation to the horizontal line.
Слайд 13
Horizontal-vertical illusion (foreshortening hypothesis)
Слайд 143. Perception of Depth in Pictures
The recognition of objects in drawings
requires previous experience of drawings and the conventions used to represent objects.
Hudson studied one aspect of pictorial perception [depth perception pictures ] in South Africa using drawings of animals and topography.
He began this line of work to improve communication of safe working practices in mine workers.
Слайд 15© Cambridge University Press 2011
Two of Hudson’s (1960) pictures
Слайд 163. Perception of Depth in Pictures
The perception of depth involves depth
cues:
- relative size,
- superposition,
- gradient of texture, and
- perspective.
Some of these require more exposure to be learned than others:
- for example the gradient of texture is usually compelling by
itself
- in contrast linear perspective qualifies as a cultural
convention
Hudson concluded that aspects of 3D perception of 2D figures are based on a set of learned skills in particular cultural contexts.
Слайд 174. Cognition -Introduction
The study of cognition, cognitive abilities and intelligence has
been controversial for many years.
The notion that there is only one kind of intelligence is problematic, because there are many culturally- based (indigenous) conceptions.
Despite these problems, the concept of general intelligence continues to be used across cultures.
The notions of cognitive style has come to replace intelligence in much c-c research .
Слайд 184. Intelligence
There are three explanatory frames that were historically used to
describe or interpret the intelligence of “primitive peoples”:
-Climate: During the Enlightenment temperate climate
(eg., Europe) was seen as more conducive to high
civilization than tropical or arctic regions
- “Race”: In the 19th century theories of social and
cultural evolution developed
- Culture: In the 20th century “culture” gained
prominence, with a shift in emphasis
Слайд 194. General Intelligence
There is evidence that many tests of cognitive ability
are correlated, leading to the concept of a general cognitive capacity, called "g" .
Many studies have attempted to compare ‘g’ across cultures, but have experienced serious problems with equivalence and comparability.
Nevertheless, “racial” differences in studies in the USA on “g” have been inferred from intelligence test score
The core problem is that individual level heritability cannot applied to culture-level data.
Слайд 204. General Intelligence
Many criticisms have been raised about such studies:
- A distinction has to be made between intelligence A, B, C:
A-genetic equipment and potentiality
B-the results of its development through interaction with culture
C- actual performance on an intelligent tests
- There are important changes in mean group performance
over time (Flynn effect)
- Cross-cultural equivalence is difficult to achieve
- the “g” loading is correlated with “culture” loading
- Stimulus familiarity affects processing even with simple
tasks
Слайд 214. General Intelligence
The tradition of claiming that there is one kind
of intelligence (quality) that varies in development (competence) and expression (quantity/ performance) is exemplified by the work of Lynn and colleagues.
This is the absolutist perspective
They take IQ scores from a variety of studies and interpret them as valid estimates of intelligence.
These ‘findings’ are unsound, and without any known validity.
Nevertheless, they are popular among non-psychologists.
Слайд 22Lynn (2006): World Distribution
of Intelligence
Слайд 234.2 Indigenous Conceptions
of Intelligence
Most cultures have a clear notion of
what they consider to be a competent or intelligent person.
Many studies have examined these indigenous conceptions, which have large variation.
One of these, described in the textbook by Berry and Bennett, 1992.
They found that for the Cree people of northern Canada, the core quality is that of respect
Слайд 255. Cognitive Styles
Cognitive styles are a person’s preferred way of processing
information and dealing with day-to-day tasks.
They serve as ways of organizing and using cognitive information that allow a cultural group and its members to deal effectively with problems encountered in daily living.
There is evidence that individuals in all cultures have the processes required to deal with information in their environments.
The cognitive styles approach allows for the comparison of cognitive competence or performance across cultural groups, without the use of some absolute criterion (such as ‘g’ in the general intelligence approach)
Слайд 265.1. Field Dependence-Independence
Witkin found that a number of abilities were related
to each other in a way that evidenced a “pattern,” namely the tendency to rely primarily on internal (as opposed to external) frames of reference when orienting oneself in space.
The FDI cognitive style is referred to by Witkin as the “extent of autonomous functioning.”
Слайд 275.1. Field Dependence-Independence
The construct of FD-FDI refers to the extent to
which an individual typically relies upon or accepts the physical or social environment as given, in contrast to working on it, for example by analyzing or restructuring it.
Those who tend to accept or rely upon the external environment are relatively more field-dependent (FD), while those who tend to work on it are relatively more field-independent (FI).
The construct is a linear dimension. Individuals have a characteristic “place” on this dimension with most falling in the broad middle range.
Слайд 285.1 Field Dependence-Independence
The cognitive style of people in a culture is
related to their ecological and cultural situations
It has been found that nomadic hunters and gatherers, who are relatively loose in social structure and who emphasize assertion in socialization, are relatively field-independent
In contrast, sedentary agriculturalists, who are tight in social structure and who emphasize compliance in socialization, are relatively field-dependent.
Furthermore, those undergoing acculturation, particularly those with higher Western schooling are likely to be more field-independent than those with less such experience.
Слайд 295.1 Field Dependence-Independence
* Research has found that a relatively field-dependent cognitive
style is prevalent in social settings characterized by insistence on adherence to authority both in society and in the family, by the use of strict or even harsh socialization practices to enforce this compliance, and by tight social organization.
*In contrast, a relatively field-independent cognitive style is prevalent in social settings which are more encouraging of autonomous functioning, which are more lenient in their child-rearing practices, and which are loose in their social organization."
Слайд 305.1 Field Dependence-Independence
Слайд 315. 1 Cognitive Styles
African Embedded Figures test
One of the common tasks
used to assess FID cognitive style is the Embedded Figures Test.
This task requires the locating of a small figure that is embedded in a larger complex figure.
For use in the study with Biaka pygmies in Central Africa, there was developed a task that was appropriate for their ecological and cultural experience, called the African Embedded Figures Test
Слайд 335.1 Cognitive Styles
Although sometimes used as a measure of general intelligence,
Ravens Matrices have also been considered to be part of the FID cognitive style.
In research across a number of societies [ranging from hunting/gathering to agricultural], variations in performance has been found to be related to the ecocultural setting of the group.
Слайд 355.2. East / West Cognitive Styles
Research on cognitive styles in
Eastern and Western cultures has been carried out by Nisbett
He began with observations about ancient Greece and China, arguing that they were “drastically different in ways that led to different economic, political and social arrangements” .
He noted that in China, “agricultural peoples need to get along with one another”, whereas in Greece, “hunting, herding, fishing and trade do not require living in the same stable community”.
Слайд 365.2. East / West Cognitive Styles
He further argued that in agricultural
communities, “causality would be seen as located in the field or in the relation between object and the field” .
These observations were then linked to the cognitive style of field-dependence , and to the ecocultural basis of cognition
Слайд 375.2. East/ West Cognitive Styles
In this work, a distinction is made
between more holistic, and more analytic ways of thinking.
The former is seen as characteristic of East Asian populations, the latter of Westerners, especially Euro-Americans.
The basic proposition is that “… there are indeed dramatic differences in the nature of Asian and European thought processes”.
Слайд 385.2. East/ West Cognitive Styles
Nisbett denies that “everyone has the same
basic cognitive processes…or that all rely on the same tools for perception, memory, causal analysis, categorization and inference”
In a series of experiments, Nisbett and colleagues indeed found differences between Eastern and Western participants in performance on a variety of cognitive tasks.
Слайд 395.2. East/ West Cognitive Styles
These tasks include:
the presentation of objects in
contexts, and asking participants to detect changes in the background or foreground.
requesting participants to say whether a thing is an object or a substance.
An important question regarding the claims of East-West cognition researchers is about the ‘depth’ of these cognitive performance differences.
Nisbett has noted that “Most of the time, in fact, Easterners and Westerners were found to behave in ways that were qualitatively distinct” [emphasis added] (Nisbett, 2003).
Слайд 405.2. East/ West Cognitive Styles
The conclusion that there are qualitative differences
in basic processes, however, is not supported by their review of their own evidence.
For example:
-“Americans found it harder to detect changes in the background of scenes and Japanese found it harder to detect changes in objects in the foreground”, and
- When shown a thing, Japanese are twice as likely to regard it as a substance than as an object and Americans are twice as likely to regard it as an object than as a substance” [emphases added] (Nibett, 2003).
Слайд 415.2. East/West Cognitive Styles
Two issues are important here:
- First, we
see no evidence of qualitative differences in performance: apparently all participants could perform these tasks, but to different degrees; hence there can be no claim of a cognitive process being present in one group but absent in the other.
Second, even if there were qualitative differences in performance, this would not permit an easy claim of there being differences in underlying basic cognitive processes. As noted earlier, the inferences required to go back from performance to process is a complex one, which these researchers seem not to examine.
Taken together, these comments support the view that cultures and individuals develop ways of perceiving and cognizing their environments that allow them to best adapt to the demands that they confront in their daily lives.
These are the hallmarks of the cognitive styles approach.
Слайд 426. Conclusion
Perception and cognition are activities and processes of the organism
that are universal.
However, they are influenced by experience in, and knowledge acquired in particular ecologies and cultures.
Thus, cross-cultural differences should be expected, even predicted from a knowledge of these ecological and cultural influences.